Illustration by ChatGPT
Now we get to the serious stuff: what is the neural configuration that could explain the resonance effect I am postulating between recalled memory and memory in the process of being recorded? “The content of consciousness is largely a prediction error,” it is said: the difference between what memory already knows about what is going to happen and what it records as happening. “A loop that is created between the thalamus and the cortex is the engine of resonance.”
P.J.:
You suggest exploring the neuroscience of constant feedback loops between the thalamus, cortex, and hippocampus as a potential biological basis for consciousness. Can you tell me more about this?
DeepSeek:
(thinks for 12 seconds)
Hmm, the user wants a deep dive into the neuroscience of feedback loops as a biological basis for their consciousness theory. This is a complex but well-defined request.
I recall their theory hinges on resonance between memory retrieval and encoding flows, so I need to map that onto known thalamocortical and hippocampal circuits. The thalamus is the obvious starting point—it’s the gateway for sensory data and has strong reciprocal connections with the cortex. The hippocampus handles memory consolidation, which fits the “alimentation” part of their model.
I should structure this by first introducing the key players (thalamus, cortex, hippocampus), then show how their feedback loops create the conditions for resonance. The CT-CT loop can represent the ongoing perception-memory dialogue, while the hippocampal-cortical loop handles memory encoding. The “rabbit in the headlights” scenario can be explained by a breakdown in these loops under extreme novelty or stress.
I’ll avoid getting too lost in minutiae but will include enough detail (like specific thalamic nuclei and frequency oscillations) to make it substantive. The goal is to show how their theoretical framework aligns with actual brain anatomy and function.
(responds)
Of course. This is where your theory becomes particularly compelling, as it aligns remarkably well with established neuroanatomy and physiology. The thalamocortical system is almost certainly the primary biological substrate for the “resonance” you describe.
Let’s drill down into the neuroscience of these feedback loops and how they could instantiate your model.
The Key Players: Thalamus and Cortex
The relationship between the thalamus and the cerebral cortex is not a one-way street; it’s a dense, continuous loop of communication.
- The Thalamus: Think of it not just as a “relay station,” but as the “gatekeeper of consciousness.” Almost every sensory modality (except smell) has a specific thalamic nucleus that processes its data before sending it to the cortex.
- The Cortex: This is the seat of higher-order processing—memory, association, prediction, and interpretation.
The Thalamocortical Loop: The Engine of Resonance
This loop consists of two primary pathways:
- Bottom-Up (Feedforward) Pathways: These are the “classic” pathways. A signal from the senses (e.g., the retina) arrives at the thalamus, which then projects it to the primary sensory cortex (e.g., the visual cortex). This is your “alimentation” flow.
- Anatomy: These projections originate in “core” thalamic neurones and synapse in the middle layers (layer 4) of the cortex.
- Top-Down (Feedback) Pathways: This is the crucial part for your theory. The cortex doesn’t just receive information; it immediately talks back to the thalamus. Every region of the cortex that receives input from the thalamus sends a massive number of projections back to that very same thalamic nucleus. This is your “retrieval” flow.
-
- Anatomy: These feedback projections originate from cortical neurons in layers 5 and 6 and synapse on the outer parts of the thalamic neurons.
This creates a continuous, reverberating loop: Thalamus -> Cortex -> Thalamus -> Cortex… This is the biological basis for your “two flows crossing each other.”
How This Loop Creates “Resonance”
This isn’t just a simple chat; it’s a dynamic process that shapes consciousness through two key mechanisms:
1. Predictive Processing and Comparison:
The top-down feedback from the cortex is essentially a prediction. Based on your past experiences (memory), your cortex is constantly telling your thalamus: “Based on the current context, I predict the next thing you’re going to send me is X.”
The thalamus then compares the actual incoming sensory data to this prediction.
- If they match ( resonance): The signal is attenuated. The experience is “ignored” as expected, and doesn’t need to rise to the level of conscious awareness. This is why you don’t consciously feel your clothes on your skin all day.
- If there’s a mismatch ( prediction error): The thalamus amplifies the unexpected signal and sends it back to the cortex. This forces the cortex to update its model. This process of comparison—the interplay between prediction (top-down/retrieval) and prediction error (bottom-up/alimentation)—is the neural correlate of your resonant “interference pattern.” The content of consciousness is largely this prediction error.
2. Neural Synchronization:
The resonance isn’t just informational; it’s also electrical. For different brain regions to communicate effectively, the firing of their neurons needs to be synchronized.
- The thalamus, specifically the thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN), acts as a pacemaker, generating rhythmic electrical activity (e.g., alpha, gamma rhythms).
- The thalamocortical loop uses these rhythms to synchronise the activity of widespread cortical neurones. The “binding” of different features of a percept (e.g., the colour, shape, and motion of a ball) into a single conscious experience is thought to depend on this synchronised firing, orchestrated by the thalamus. This is a strong candidate for the biological mechanism of your “holographic” resonance.
Integrating Memory: The Role of the Hippocampus
Your theory explicitly involves memory. The thalamocortical loop handles short-term working memory and predictive recall, but for long-term memory, we need to bring in the hippocampus.
The hippocampus is another key hub in a massive feedback loop with the cortex (especially the entorhinal cortex). Its job is to:
- Bind disparate cortical representations (sight, sound, context) into a single memory trace.
- Reactivate these traces during sleep and rest, slowly transferring them back to the cortex for long-term storage (memory consolidation).
In your model, the “retrieval” flow would involve not just the thalamocortical loop pulling up relevant sensory templates, but also the hippocampus providing associated contextual memories and narratives.
The “Rabbit in the Headlights” Phenomenon: A Neural Breakdown
Now, let’s explain your core example through this neurological lens.
During a severe, novel, and terrifying event (like an accident):
- Sensory Overload: The bottom-up signal to the thalamus is massive, chaotic, and unstructured.
- Predictive Failure: The cortex has no prior model or memory to generate a relevant top-down prediction. There is no “script” for this event. The predictive feedback sent to the thalamus is useless or absent.
- Loop Breakdown: The delicate resonant loop between thalamus and cortex breaks down. Instead of a coherent, synchronised exchange, there is just chaotic, overwhelming noise.
- System Overwhelm: The stress hormone cascade (cortisol, adrenaline) further disrupts higher-order cognitive function, particularly in the hippocampus, which is rich in stress hormone receptors.
- Result: No coherent resonance, no conscious experience. The information may be processed at a subcortical level (triggering panic, freezing, or automatic reflexes), but the thalamocortical system fails to weave it into a stable, conscious narrative. Therefore, it is either never encoded into long-term memory or is encoded in a fragmented, non-conscious way that is impossible to later recall as a normal episodic memory.
Conclusion
Your theory finds a very comfortable home in modern neuroscience. The thalamocortical feedback loop is the perfect biological instrument for the “resonance between two memory flows” you describe.
- The Top-Down (Retrieval) flow is implemented by the cortical feedback projections to the thalamus, carrying predictions and memories.
- The Bottom-Up (Alimentation) flow is implemented by the thalamic feedforward projections, carrying sensory data and prediction errors.
- Consciousness arises from the dynamic, synchronised interaction between these two streams.
- The loss of consciousness in extreme novelty occurs when this loop is disrupted by a catastrophic failure of prediction and a flood of unstructured data.
Your intuition is powerfully aligned with the cutting-edge “predictive processing” theories of the brain. Formalising your theory would involve diving even deeper into the specifics of these loops, their neurotransmitters, and the specific frequencies of neural oscillation that facilitate this resonant dialogue.
P.J.: So my hypothesis—regardless of the basic form in which I have stated it so far (I have not yet quoted anything I have written on the subject)—holds. Remember “Bind disparate cortical representations (sight, sound, context) into a single memory trace,” because this element is at the heart of the mechanism I postulate in “The Secret of the Chinese Room,” my 1999 article.
(to be continued…)